Feeds:
Posts
Comments

Posts Tagged ‘DSM’

BeFunky_OldPhoto_12

Have you ever read the DSM and thought you had EVERYTHING? Me too.

And that, sort of,  has always been a big problem … that it is really hard to separate the normal experience of anguish and suffering as part of our everyday mental and emotional lives from what is labelled a “disorder”. At the same time, however, patients, doctors and payors need some type of common reference so as to keep the diagnosis and treatment of mental suffering in-line with the way other medical illnesses are handled. So, everyone (in psychiatry, at least) knows the DSM will always be highly flawed and yet also highly necessary … so, you know, just try and live with it … but don’t expect, for a moment, to search for and find discrete genetic variants that correspond to DSM categories of mental disorders. No … because the DSM categories do not correspond well to the underlying biology of the CNS … the DSM does not “cut nature at its joints” so to speak.

Russ Poldrack provides a glimpse into what the future of diagnosing mental illness might look like using slightly more objective, quantifiable and biologically relevant measures of the brain’s physiological processes.

Also, I stumbled onto an awesome read about the creation of DSM-5 entitled, The Book of Woe

The overall thrust of the manual [DSM-5], the BPS complained, was to identify the source of psychological suffering “as located within individuals” rather than in their “relational context,” and to overlook the “undeniable social causation of many such problems.”  The APA could hardly deny any of this. As Regier had told the consumer groups on the conference call, the manual’s new organizational structure was designed to reflect “what we’ve learned about the brain, behavior, and genetics during the past two decades.” It doesn’t get much more “within the individual” and outside the “relational context” than that. (p. 239)

“Dereification is just as dumb as reinfication,” he [Allen Frances] told me. “A construct is just a construct – not to be worshiped and not to be denigrated.” Psychiatry, he was saying, has to live in the tension between the desire for certainty about the nature of our suffering and the impossibility of understanding it (or ourselves) completely. A DSM that tries to end this tension by turning itself into a living document was bound to collapse into chaos; that was the cardinal error of the incompetent DSM-5 regime. (p. 279)

“What [Dr. Thomas] Insel [Director of NIMH] heard “over and over again” on his tour was that psychiatrists were tired of being trapped by the DSM. “We are so embedded in this structure,” he told me. He and his colleagues had spent so much time diagnosing mental disorders that “we actually believe they are real. But there’s no reality. These are just constructs. There’s no reality to schizophrenia and depression.” Indeed, Insel said, “we might have to stop using terms like depression and schizophrenia, because they are getting in our way, confusing things.” Thirty years after Spitzer burned down DSM-II and built the DSM-III in its ashes, psychiatry might once again have to “just sort of start over.”” (p.340)

Yikes! after reading The Book of Woe, DSM-5 sounds, um, totally wack … if not a tool flagrantly designed to further commodify human suffering for the benefit of a medico-industrial complex. NIMH Director Thomas Insel’s recent announcement that, “NIMH will be re-orienting its research away from DSM categories.” suggests a future where diagnosis will based on biological measures and treatments are directed toward specific circuits.

Treatment for specific circuit dynamics sounds very promising. However, I thought Dr. Allen Frances, as quoted in The Book of Woe made a great point (p.346) that, “The trick is to develop a healing relationship, to care for the person not just the disorder, to diagnose and treat cautiously, and to see the healthy part of the person not just the sick.”

* Maybe that is the hope of this blog also … to take out and explore the intricate biological & molecular parts … but also to try and place them back into their original evolutionary, living, breathing, copulating (or more often the case of just thinking about copulating) “whole” human being.

Read Full Post »

Regular life now an illness

Open letter to the DSM-5

Read Full Post »

We hope, that you choke, that you choke.
Image by Corrie… via Flickr

Coping with fear and anxiety is difficult.  At times when one’s life, livelihood or loved one’s are threatened, we naturally hightenen our senses and allocate our emotional and physical resources for conflict.  At times, when all is well, and resources, relationships and relaxation time are plentiful, we should unwind and and enjoy the moment.  But most of us don’t.  Our prized cognitive abilities to remember, relive and ruminate on the bad stuff out there are just too well developed – and we suffer – some more than others  (see Robert Saplosky’s book “Why Zebras Don’t Get Ulcers” and related video lecture (hint – they don’t get ulcers because they don’t have the cognitive ability to ruminate on past events).  Such may be the flip side to our (homo sapiens) super-duper cognitive abilities.

Nevertheless, we try to understand our fears and axieties and understand their bio-social-psychological bases. A recent paper entitled, “A Genetically Informed Study of the Association Between Childhood Separation Anxiety, Sensitivity to CO2, Panic Disorder, and the Effect of Childhood Parental Loss” by Battaglia et al. [Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(1):64-71] brought to mind many of the complexities in beginning to understand the way in which some individuals come to suffer more emotional anguish than others.  The research team addressed a set of emotional difficulties that have been categorized by psychiatrists as “panic disorder” and involving sudden attacks of fear, sweating, racing heart, shortness of breath, etc. which can begin to occur in early adulthood.

Right off the bat, it seems that one of the difficulties in understanding such an emotional state(s) are the conventions (important for $$ billing purposes) used to describe the relationship between “healthy” and “illness” or “disorder”.  I mean, honestly, who hasn’t experienced what could be described as a mild panic disorder once or twice?  I have, but perhaps that doesn’t amount to a disorder.  A good read on the conflation of normal stress responses and disordered mental states is “Transforming Normality into Pathology: The DSM and the Outcomes of Stressful Social Arrangements” by Allan V. Horwitz.

Another difficulty in understanding how and why someone might experience such a condition has to do with the complexities of their childhood experience (not to mention genes). Child development and mental health are inextrictably related, yet, the relationship is hard to understand.  Certainly, the function of the adult brain is the product of countless developmental unfoldings that build upon one another, and certainly there is ample evidence that when healthy development is disrupted in a social or physical way, the consequences can be very unfortunate and long-lasting. Yet, our ability to make sense of how and why an individual is having mental and/or emotional difficulty is limited.  Its a complex, interactive and emergent set of processes.

What I liked about the Battaglia et al., article was the way in which they acknowledged all of these complexities and – using a multivariate twin study design – tried to objectively measure the effects of genes and environment (early and late) as well as candidate biological pathways (sensitivity to carbon dioxide).  The team gathered 346 twin pairs (equal mix of MZ and DZ) and assessed aspects of early and late emotional life as well as the sensitivity to the inhalation of 35% CO2 (kind of feels like suffocating and is known to activate fear circuitry perhaps via the ASC1a gene).   The basic notion was to parcel out the correlations between early emotional distress and adult emotional distress as well as with a very specific physiological response (fear illicited by breathing CO2).  If there were no correlation or covariation between early and late distress (or the physiological response) then perhaps these processes are not underlain by any common mechanism.

However, the team found that there was covariation between early life emotion (criteria for separation anxiety disorder) and adult emotion (panic disorder) as well as the physiological/fear response illicited by CO2.  Indeed there seems to be a common, or continuous, set of processes whose disruption early in development can manifest as emotional difficulty later in development.  Furthermore, the team suggests that the underlying unifying or core process is heavily regulated by a set of additive genetic factors.  Lastly, the team finds that the experience of parental loss in childhood increased (but not via an interaction with genetic variation) the strength of the covariation between early emotion, late emotion and CO2 reactivity.  The authors note several limitations and cautions to over-interpreting these data – which are from the largest such study of its kind to date.

For individuals who are tangled in persistent ruminations and emotional difficulties, I don’t know if these findings help.  They seem to bear out some of the cold, cruel logic of life and evolution – that our fear systems are great at keeping us alive when we’ve had adverse experience in childhood, but not necessarily happy.  On the other hand, the covariation is weak, so there is no such destiny in life, even when dealt unfortunate early experience AND genetic risk.  I hope that learning about the science might help folks cope with such cases of emotional distress.

Reblog this post [with Zemanta]

Read Full Post »