Feeds:
Posts
Comments

Posts Tagged ‘Genomic imprinting’

Where's Waldo in Google Maps?
Image by Si1very via Flickr

In an earlier post on Williams Syndrome, we delved into the notion that sometimes a genetic variant can lead to enhanced function – such as certain social behaviors in the case of WS.  A mechanism that is thought to underlie this phenomenon has to do with the way in which information processing in the brain is widely distributed and that sometimes a gene variant can impact one processing pathway, while leaving another pathway intact, or even upregulated.  In the case of Williams Syndrome a relatively intact ventral stream (“what”) processing but disrupted dorsal stream (“where”) processing leads to weaker projections to the frontal cortex and amygdala which may facilitate gregarious and prosocial (a lack of fear and inhibition) behavior.  Other developmental disabilities may differentially disrupt these 2 visual information processing pathways.  For instance, developmental dyspraxia contrasts with WS as it differentially disrupts the ventral stream processing pathway.

A recent paper by Woodcock and colleagues in their article, “Dorsal and ventral stream mediated visual processing in genetic subtypes of Prader–Willi syndrome” [doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.019] ask how another developmental disability – Prader-Willi syndrome – might differentially influence the development of these information processing pathways.  PWS arises from the lack of expression (via deletion or uniparental disomy) of a cluster of paternally expressed genes in the 15q11-13 region (normally the gene on the maternally inherited chromosome is silent, or imprintedrelated post here).  By comparing PWS children to matched controls, the team reports evidence showing that PWS children who carry the deletion are slightly more impaired in a task that depends on the dorsal “where” pathway whilst some sparing or relative strength in the ventral “what” pathway.

Reblog this post [with Zemanta]

Read Full Post »

Human chromosome 15
Image via Wikipedia

One way to organize the great and growing body of research into autism is via a sort-of  ‘top-down’ vs. ‘bottom-up’ perspective.  From the ‘top-down’ one can read observational research that carefully catalogs the many & varied social and cognitive attributes that are associated with autism.  Often times, these behavioral studies are coupled with neurochemical or neuroimaging studies that test whether variation in such biomarkers is correlated with aspects of autism.  In this manner, the research aims to dig down into the physiology and biochemistry of the developing brain to find out what is different and what differences might predict the onset of autistic traits.  At the deepest biological level – the bedrock, so to speak – are a number of genetic variations that have been correlated with autism.  These genetic variants permit another research strategy – a ‘bottom-up’ strategy that allows investigators to ask, “what goes wrong when we manipulate this genetic variant?”  While proponents of each strategy are painfully aware of the limitations of their own strategy – oft on the barbed-end of commentary from the other side – it is especially exciting when the ‘top-down’ and ‘bottom-up’ methods find themselves meeting in the agreement in the middle.

So is the case with Nakatani et al., “Abnormal Behavior in a Chromosome- Engineered Mouse Model for Human 15q11-13 Duplication Seen in Autism” [doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.04.024] who created a mouse that carries a 6.3 megabase duplication of a region in the mouse that luckily happens to be remarkably conserved in terms of gene identity and order with the 15q11-13 region in humans – a region that, when duplicated, is found in about 5% of cases with autism.  [click here for maps of mouse human synteny/homology on human chr15] Thus the team was able to engineer mice with the duplication and ask, “what goes wrong?” and “does it resemble autism in any kind of meaningful way (afterall these are mice we’re dealing with)?

Well, the results are rather astounding to me.  Most amazing is the expression of a small nucleoar RNA (snoRNA) – SNORD115 (mouse-HBII52) – that function in the nucleolus of the cell, and plays a role in the alternative splicing of exon Vb of the 5HT2C receptor.  The team then found that the editing of 5HTR2C was altered in the duplication mice and also that Ca++ signalling was increased when the 5HTR2C receptors were stimulated in the duplication mice (compared to controls).  Thus, a role for altered serotonin function – which has been a longstanding finding in the ‘topdown’ approach – was met midway and affirmed by this ‘bottom-up’ approach!  Also included in the paper are descriptions of the abberant social behaviors of the mice via a 3-chambered social interaction test where duplication mice were rather indifferent to a stranger mouse (wild-type mice often will hang out with each other).

Amazing stuff!

Another twist to the story is the way in which the 15q11-13 region displays a phenomenon known as genomic-imprinting, whereby only the mother or the father’s portion of the chromosome is expressed.  For example, the authors show that the mouse duplication is ‘maternally imprinted’ meaning that that pups do not express the copy of the duplication that comes from the mother (its expression is shut down via epigenetic mechanisms that involve – wait for itsnoRNAs!)  so the effects that they report are only from mice who obtained the duplication from their fathers.  So, if you by chance were wondering why its so tough to sort out the genetic basis of autism – here’s one reason why.  On top of this, the 5HTR2C gene is located on the X-chromosome which complicates the story even more in terms of sorting out the inheritance of the disorder.

Further weird & wild is the fact that the UBE3A gene (paternally imprinted) and the genetic cause of Angelman Syndrome sits in this region – as does the SNRPN gene (maternally imprinted) which encodes a protein that influences alternative RNA splicing and also gives rise to Prader-Willi syndrome.  Thus, this tiny region of the genome, which carries so-called “small” RNAs can influence a multitude of developmental disabilities.  Certainly, a region of the genome that merits further study!!

Reblog this post [with Zemanta]

Read Full Post »